Science Magazine

Friday, July 30, 2004: VOL 305 30 JULY 2004 599 CREDITCORBIS

By Mary Beckman

www​.sci​encemag​.org

Crime, Culpability and the Adolescent Brain

This fall, the U.S. Supreme Court will con­sid­er whether cap­i­tal crimes by teenagers under 18 should get the death sen­tence; the case for lenien­cy is based in part on brain studies.

When he was 17 years old, Christopher Simmons per­suad­ed a younger friend to help him rob a woman, tie her up with elec­tri­cal cable and duct tape, and throw her over a bridge. He was con­vict­ed of mur­der and sen­tenced to death by a Missouri court in 1994. In a whip­saw of legal pro­ceed­ings, the Missouri Supreme Court set the sen­tence aside last year. Now 27, Simmons could again face exe­cu­tion: The state of Missouri has appealed to have the death penal­ty rein­stat­ed. The U.S. Supreme Court will hear the case in October, and its deci­sion could well rest on neurobiology.

At issue is whether 16- and 17-year-olds who com­mit cap­i­tal offens­es can be exe­cut­ed or whether this would be cru­el and unusu­al pun­ish­ment, banned by the Constitution’s eighth amend­ment. In a joint brief filed on 19 July, eight med­ical and men­tal health orga­ni­za­tions includ­ing the American Medical Association cite a sheaf of devel­op­men­tal biol­o­gy and behav­ioral lit­er­a­ture to sup­port their argu­ment that ado­les­cent brains have not reached their full adult poten­tial. Capacities rel­e­vant to crim­i­nal respon­si­bil­i­ty are still devel­op­ing when you’re 16 or 17 years old,” says psy­chol­o­gist Laurence Steinberg of the American Psychological Association, which joined the brief sup­port­ing Simmons. Adds physi­cian David Fassler, spokesper­son for the American Psychiatric Association (APA) and the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, the argu­ment does not excuse vio­lent crim­i­nal behav­ior, but it’s an impor­tant fac­tor for courts to con­sid­er” when wield­ing a pun­ish­ment as extreme and irre­versible as death.”

The Supreme Court has addressed some of these issues before. In 1988, it held that it was uncon­sti­tu­tion­al to exe­cute con­victs under 16, but it ruled in 1989 that states were with­in their rights to put 16- and 17-year-old crim­i­nals to death. Thirteen years lat­er, it decid­ed that men­tal­ly retard­ed peo­ple should­n’t be exe­cut­ed because they have a reduced capac­i­ty for rea­son­ing, judg­ment, and con­trol of their impuls­es,” even though they gen­er­al­ly know right from wrong (see side­bar on p. 599). That is the stan­dard Simmons’s lawyers now want the court to extend to every­one under 18.

Cruel and unusual?

Simmons’s lawyers argue that ado­les­cents are not as moral­ly cul­pa­ble as adults and there­fore should not be sub­ject to the death penal­ty. They claim that this view reflects world­wide chang­ing stan­dards of decen­cy,” a trend that has been rec­og­nized in many U.S. courts. Today, 31 states and the fed­er­al gov­ern­ment have banned the juve­nile death penal­ty. The lat­est to do so, Wyoming and South Dakota, con­sid­ered brain devel­op­ment research in their deci­sions. Putting a 17-year-old to death for cap­i­tal crimes is cru­el and unusu­al pun­ish­ment, accord­ing to this rea­son­ing. What was cru­el and unusu­al when the Constitution was writ­ten is dif­fer­ent from today. We don’t put peo­ple in stock­ades now,” says Stephen Harper, a lawyer with the Juvenile Justice Center of the American Bar Association (ABA), which also signed an ami­cus curi­ae brief. These stan­dards mark the progress of a civilized society.”

The defense is focus­ing on the cul­pa­bil­i­ty of juve­niles and whether their brains are as capa­ble of impulse con­trol, deci­sion-mak­ing, and rea­son­ing as adult brains are,” says law pro­fes­sor Steven Drizin of Northwestern University in Chicago. And some brain researchers answer with a resound­ing no.” The brain’s frontal lobe, which exer­cis­es restraint over impul­sive behav­ior, does­n’t begin to mature until 17 years of age,” says neu­ro­sci­en­tist Ruben Gur of the University of Pennsylvania in Philadelphia. The very part of the brain that is judged by the legal sys­tem process comes on board late.”

But oth­er researchers hes­i­tate to apply sci­en­tists’ opin­ions to set­tle moral and legal ques­tions. Although brain research should prob­a­bly take a part in pol­i­cy debate, it’s dam­ag­ing to use sci­ence to sup­port essen­tial­ly moral stances, says neu­ro­sci­en­tist Paul Thompson of the University of California, Los Angeles (UCLA).

Shades of gray

Structurally, the brain is still grow­ing and matur­ing dur­ing ado­les­cence, begin­ning its final push around 16 or 17, many brain-imag­ing researchers agree. Some say that growth max­es out at age 20. Others, such as Jay Giedd of the National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH) in Bethesda, Maryland, con­sid­er 25 the age at which brain mat­u­ra­tion peaks. Various types of brain scans and anatom­ic dis­sec­tions show that as teens age, dis­or­dered-look­ing neu­ron cell bod­ies known as gray mat­ter recede, and neu­ron pro­jec­tions cov­ered in a pro­tec­tive fat­ty sheath, called white mat­ter, take over. In 1999, Giedd and col­leagues showed that just before puber­ty, chil­dren have a growth spurt of gray mat­ter. This is fol­lowed by mas­sive prun­ing” in which about 1% of gray mat­ter is pared down each year dur­ing the teen years, while the total vol­ume of white mat­ter ramps up. This process is thought to shape the brain’s neur­al con­nec­tions for adult­hood, based on experience.

In argu­ing for lenien­cy, Simmons’s sup­port­ers cite some of the lat­est research that points to the imma­tu­ri­ty of youth­ful brains, such as a May study of chil­dren and teens, led by NIMH’s Nitin Gogtay. The team fol­lowed 13 indi­vid­u­als between the ages of 4 and 21, per­form­ing mag­net­ic res­o­nance imag­ing (MRI) every 2 years to track changes in the phys­i­cal struc­ture of brain tis­sue. As pre­vi­ous research had sug­gest­ed, the frontal lobes matured last. Starting from the back of the head, we see a wave of brain change mov­ing for­ward into the front of the brain like a for­est fire,” says UCLA’s Thompson, a co-author. The brain changes con­tin­ued up to age 21, the old­est per­son they exam­ined. It’s quite pos­si­ble that the brain mat­u­ra­tion peaks after age 21,” he adds.

The images showed a rapid con­ver­sion from gray to white mat­ter. Thompson says that researchers debate whether teens are actu­al­ly los­ing tis­sue when the gray mat­ter dis­ap­pears, trim­ming con­nec­tions, or just coat­ing gray mat­ter with insu­la­tion. Imaging does­n’t pro­vide high enough res­o­lu­tion to dis­tin­guish among the pos­si­bil­i­ties, he notes: Right now we can image chunks of mil­lions of neu­rons, but we can’t look at indi­vid­ual cells.” A type of spec­troscopy that picks out N‑acetylaspartate, a chem­i­cal found only in neu­rons, shows promise in help­ing to set­tle the issue. In addi­tion to grow­ing vol­ume, brain stud­ies doc­u­ment an increase in the orga­ni­za­tion of white mat­ter dur­ing ado­les­cence. The joint brief cites a 1999 study by Tomás Paus of McGill University in Montreal and col­leagues that used struc­tur­al MRI to show that neu­ronal tracts con­nect­ing dif­fer­ent regions of the brain thick­ened as they were coat­ed with a pro­tec­tive sheath of myelin dur­ing ado­les­cence (Science, 19 March 1999, p. 1908).

In 2002, anoth­er study revealed that these tracts gained in direc­tion­al­i­ty as well. Relying on dif­fu­sion ten­sor MRI, which fol­lows the direc­tion that water trav­els, Vincent Schmithorst of the Children’s Hospital Medical Center in Cincinnati, Ohio, and col­leagues watched the brain orga­nize itself in 33 chil­dren and teens from age 5 to 18. During ado­les­cence, the tracts fun­neled up from the spinal tract, through the brain­stem, and into motor regions. Another linked the two major lan­guage areas. The brain is get­ting more orga­nized and dense with age,” Schmithorst says.

Don’t look at the light

Adults behave dif­fer­ent­ly not just because they have dif­fer­ent brain struc­tures, accord­ing to Gur and oth­ers, but because they use the struc­tures in a dif­fer­ent way. A ful­ly devel­oped frontal lobe curbs impuls­es com­ing from oth­er parts of the brain, Gur explains: If you’ve been insult­ed, your emo­tion­al brain says, Kill,’ but your frontal lobe says you’re in the mid­dle of a cock­tail par­ty, so let’s respond with a cutting remark.’ ”

As it matures, the ado­les­cent brain slow­ly reor­ga­nizes how it inte­grates infor­ma­tion com­ing from the nether regions. Using func­tion­al MRI-which lights up sites in the brain that are active-com­bined with sim­ple tests, neu­ro­sci­en­tist Beatriz Luna of the University of Pittsburgh has found that the brain switch­es from rely­ing heav­i­ly on local regions in child­hood to more dis­trib­u­tive and col­lab­o­ra­tive inter­ac­tions among dis­tant regions in adulthood.

One of the meth­ods Luna uses to probe brain activ­i­ty is the anti­sac­cade” test: a sim­pli­fied mod­el of real-life respons­es designed to deter­mine how well the pre­frontal cor­tex gov­erns the more prim­i­tive parts of the brain. Subjects focus on a cross on a screen and are told that the cross will dis­ap­pear and a light will show up. They are told not to look at the light, which is dif­fi­cult because the whole brain­stem is wired to look at lights,” says Luna.

Adolescents can pre­vent them­selves from peek­ing at the light, but in doing so they rely on brain regions dif­fer­ent from those adults use. In 2001, Luna and col­leagues showed that ado­les­cents’ pre­frontal cor­tices were con­sid­er­ably more active than adults’ in this test. Adults also used areas in the cere­bel­lum impor­tant for tim­ing and learn­ing and brain regions that pre­pare for the task at hand.

These results sup­port oth­er evi­dence show­ing that teens’ impulse con­trol is not on a par with adults’. In work in press in Child Development, Luna found that vol­un­teers aged 14 years and old­er per­form just as well on the task as adults, but they rely main­ly on the frontal lobe’s pre­frontal cor­tex, where­as adults exhib­it a more com­plex response. The ado­les­cent is using slight­ly dif­fer­ent brain mech­a­nisms to achieve the goal,” says Luna.

Although the work is not cit­ed in the brief, Luna says it clear­ly shows that ado­les­cents can­not be viewed at the same lev­el as adults.”

Processing fear

Other stud­ies-based on the amyg­dala, a brain region that process­es emo­tions, and research on risk aware­ness-indi­cate that teenagers are more prone to errat­ic behav­ior than adults. Abigail Baird and Deborah Yurgelun-Todd of Harvard Medical School in Boston and oth­ers asked teens in a 1999 study to iden­ti­fy the emo­tion they per­ceive in pic­tures of faces. As expect­ed, func­tion­al MRI showed that in both ado­les­cents and adults, the amyg­dala burst with activ­i­ty when pre­sent­ed with a face show­ing fear. But the pre­frontal cor­tex did­n’t blaze in teens as it did in adults, sug­gest­ing that emo­tion­al respons­es have lit­tle inhi­bi­tion. In addi­tion, the teens kept mis­tak­ing fear­ful expres­sions for anger or other emotions.

Baird, now at Dartmouth College in Hanover, New Hampshire, says that sub­se­quent exper­i­ments showed that in teenagers the pre­frontal cor­tex buzzes when they view expres­sions of peo­ple they know. Also, the chil­dren iden­ti­fied the cor­rect emo­tion more than 95% of the time, an improve­ment of 20% over the pre­vi­ous work. The key dif­fer­ence between the results, says Baird, is that ado­les­cents pay atten­tion to things that mat­ter to them but have dif­fi­cul­ty inter­pret­ing images that are unfa­mil­iar or seem remote in time. Teens shown a dis­co-era pic­ture in pre­vi­ous stud­ies would say, Oh, he’s freaked out because he’s stuck in the 70s,” she says. Teens are painful­ly aware of emo­tions, she notes.

But teens are real­ly bad at the kind of think­ing that requires look­ing into the future to see the results of actions, a char­ac­ter­is­tic that feeds increased risk-tak­ing. Baird sug­gests: Ask some­one, How would you like to get roller skates and skate down some real­ly big steps?” Adults know what might hap­pen at the bot­tom and would be wary. But teens don’t see things the same way, because they have trou­ble gen­er­at­ing hypothe­ses of what might hap­pen,” says Baird, part­ly because they don’t have access to the many expe­ri­ences that adults do. The abil­i­ty to do so emerges between 15 and 18 years of age, she the­o­rizes in an upcom­ing issue of the Proceedings of the Royal Society of London.

Luna points out that the tumul­tuous nature of ado­les­cent brains is nor­mal: This tran­si­tion in ado­les­cence is not a dis­ease or an impair­ment. It’s an extreme­ly adap­tive way to make an adult.” She spec­u­lates that risk-tak­ing and low­ered inhi­bi­tions pro­vide expe­ri­ences to prune their brains.”

With all the prun­ing, myeli­na­tion, and reor­ga­ni­za­tion, an ado­les­cen­t’s brain is unsta­ble, but per­form­ing well on tests can make teens look more mature than they are. Yes, ado­les­cents can look like adults. But put stres­sors into a sys­tem that’s already frag­ile, and it can eas­i­ly revert to a less mature state,” Luna says.

The ami­cus curi­ae brief endorsed by the APA and oth­ers also describes the fragili­ty of ado­les­cence-how teens are sen­si­tive to peer pres­sure and can be com­pro­mised by a less-than-pris­tine child­hood envi­ron­ment. Abuse can affect how nor­mal­ly brains devel­op. Not sur­pris­ing­ly, every [juve­nile offend­er on death row] has been abused or neglect­ed as a kid,” says ABA attorney Harper.

Biology and behavior

Although many researchers agree that the brain, espe­cial­ly the frontal lobe, con­tin­ues to devel­op well into teen­hood and beyond, many sci­en­tists hes­i­tate to weigh in on the legal debate. Some, like Giedd, say the data just aren’t there” for them to con­fi­dent­ly tes­ti­fy to the moral or legal cul­pa­bil­i­ty of ado­les­cents in court. Neuroscientist Elizabeth Sowell of UCLA says that too lit­tle data exist to con­nect behav­ior to brain struc­ture, and imag­ing is far from being diag­nos­tic. We could­n’t do a scan on a kid and decide if they should be tried as an adult,” she says.

Harper says the reason for bringing in the scientific and medical world is not to persuade the court but to inform the court.” Fassler, who staunchly opposes the juvenile death penalty, doesn’t want to predict how the case will turn out. It will be close. I’m hopeful that the court will carefully review the scientific data and will agree with the conclusion that adolescents function in fundamentally different ways than adults.” And perhaps, advocates hope, toppling the death penalty with a scientific understanding of teenagers will spread to better ways of rehabilitating such youths.

SIDEBAR

Adolescence: Akin to Mental Retardation?

The human brain took cen­ter stage in 2002 when the U.S. Supreme Court ruled against the death penal­ty for men­tal­ly retard­ed per­sons. In that case (Atkins v. Virginia), six of the nine jus­tices agreed that exe­cut­ing a con­vict with lim­it­ed intel­lec­tu­al capac­i­ty, Daryl Atkins, would amount to cru­el and unusu­al pun­ish­ment. Instructing the state of Virginia to for­go the death penal­ty in such cas­es, Justice John Paul Stevens wrote: Because of their dis­abil­i­ties in areas of rea­son­ing, judg­ment, and con­trol of their impuls­es, [men­tal­ly retard­ed per­sons] do not act with the lev­el of moral cul­pa­bil­i­ty that char­ac­ter­izes the most seri­ous adult criminal conduct.”

When the case of Christopher Simmons, who com­mit­ted mur­der at age 17, comes before the same jus­tices in October, says law pro­fes­sor Steven Drizin of Northwestern University in Chicago, defense attor­neys hope to equate juve­nile cul­pa­bil­i­ty to that of men­tal­ly retard­ed per­sons. Juveniles func­tion very much like the men­tal­ly retard­ed. The biggest sim­i­lar­i­ty is their cog­ni­tive deficit. [Teens] may be high­ly func­tion­ing, but that does­n’t make them capa­ble of mak­ing good deci­sions,” he says. Brain and behav­ior research sup­ports that con­tention, argues Drizin, who rep­re­sents the Children and Family Justice Center at Northwestern on the ami­cus curi­ae brief for Simmons. The stan­dard of decen­cy” today is that teens do not deserve the same extreme pun­ish­ment as adults.

The Atkins deci­sion pro­vides advo­cates with a tem­plate” for what fac­tors should be laid out to deter­mine evolv­ing stan­dards of decen­cy,” says Drizin. These fac­tors include the move­ment of state leg­is­la­tures to raise the age lim­it for the death penal­ty to 18, jury ver­dicts of juve­nile offend­ers, the inter­na­tion­al con­sen­sus is on the issue, and pub­lic opin­ion polls. In 2002, the court also con­sid­ered the opin­ions of pro­fes­sion­al orga­ni­za­tions with per­ti­nent knowl­edge, which is how the brain research comes into play. Last, the jus­tices con­sid­ered evi­dence that the men­tal­ly retard­ed may be more like­ly to false­ly con­fess and be wrong­ly convicted‑a prob­lem that ado­les­cents have as well.

-M.B.