Arbitrariness

Legally Irrelevant Factors Impact Death Sentencing

The Supreme Court upheld the death penal­ty in Gregg v. Georgia based on the con­clu­sion that appro­pri­ate­ly tai­lored death penal­ty statutes could ensure that the death penal­ty is applied fair­ly. More than forty years lat­er, the death penal­ty con­tin­ues to be unpre­dictably applied to a small num­ber of defen­dants. Despite numer­ous attempts by leg­is­la­tors to craft laws that would reduce arbi­trari­ness, the death-penal­ty sys­tem has failed to mean­ing­ful­ly iden­ti­fy the worst of the worst” deeds and doers. Instead, many fac­tors oth­er than the grav­i­ty of the crime or the cul­pa­bil­i­ty of the offend­er appear to affect death sen­tences, includ­ing race, geog­ra­phy, gen­der, access to ade­quate coun­sel, and jury misperceptions.

Race

Modern stud­ies of the death penal­ty con­tin­ue to find a cor­re­la­tion between sen­tenc­ing and race. The stud­ies con­sis­tent­ly show that those who kill white vic­tims are much more like­ly to receive the death penal­ty than those who kill black vic­tims. Some stud­ies have also found bias against non-white defen­dants. Racial dis­par­i­ties in sen­tenc­ing and exe­cu­tions sug­gest that race plays a role in the appli­ca­tion of the death penalty.

In 96% of the states where there have been reviews of race and the death penal­ty, there was a pat­tern of either race-of-vic­tim or race-of-defen­dant dis­crim­i­na­tion or both. David Baldus, et al., Racial Discrimination and the Death Penalty in the Post-Furman Era: An Empirical And Legal Overview, With Recent Findings From Philadelphia, 83 Cornell L. Rev. 1638 (1998). For exam­ple, a Florida study review­ing over 10,000 cas­es con­clud­ed that those sus­pect­ed of killing whites were 3.42 times as like­ly to receive a death sen­tence as those sus­pect­ed of killing blacks.” Shatz & Dalton, Challenging the Death Penalty with Statistics: Furman, McClesky, and a Single County Case Study, 34 Cardozo L. Rev. 1227, 1246 – 47 (2013).

A study in North Carolina found that the odds of a defen­dant receiv­ing a death sen­tence were three times high­er if the per­son was con­vict­ed of killing a white per­son than if he had killed a black per­son. The study, con­duct­ed by Professors Michael Radelet and Glenn Pierce, exam­ined 15,281 homi­cides in the state between 1980 and 2007, which result­ed in 368 death sen­tences. Even after account­ing for addi­tion­al fac­tors, such as mul­ti­ple vic­tims or homi­cides accom­pa­nied with a rape, rob­bery or oth­er felony, researchers found that race was still a sig­nif­i­cant pre­dic­tor of who was sen­tenced to death. Michael L. Radelet and Glenn L. Pierce, Race and Death Sentencing in North Carolina 1980 – 2007, 89 N.C. L. Rev. 2119 (2011).

For more infor­ma­tion, see DPIC’s pages on Race and DPIC’s report The Death Penalty in Black and White: Who Lives, Who Dies, Who Decides (1998).


Gender

The num­ber of women on death row is very small and hence it is dif­fi­cult to draw any sta­tis­ti­cal con­clu­sions about gen­der bias. Death sen­tences and actu­al exe­cu­tions for female offend­ers are rare in com­par­i­son to such events for male offend­ers. In fact, women are more like­ly to be dropped out of the sys­tem the fur­ther the cap­i­tal pun­ish­ment sys­tem pro­gress­es. Women com­mit about 10% of mur­ders in the U.S., with the vic­tims typ­i­cal­ly being an acquain­tance. Women cur­rent­ly make up about 2% of those on death row and about 1% of those exe­cut­ed in the mod­ern era of the death penalty.

Several stud­ies have found that the gen­der of a mur­der vic­tim affects the like­li­hood that a defen­dant will be sen­tenced to death. For example,

  • A Florida study found that a defen­dant was 2.8 times as like­ly to be sen­tenced to death for killing a woman as for killing a man.”
  • In South Carolina, the odds were 2.19 times high­er that female vic­tim mur­ders would lead to the death penal­ty being charged than male victim murders.”
  • In California, “[i]n sin­gle-vic­tim cas­es, fac­tu­al­ly death-eli­gi­ble defen­dants con­vict­ed of killing women were more than sev­en times as like­ly to be sen­tenced to death as fac­tu­al­ly death-eli­gi­ble defen­dants who killed men.”

Source: Shatz & Dalton, Challenging the Death Penalty with Statistics: Furman, McClesky, and a Single County Case Study, 34 Cardozo L. Rev. 1227, 1251 – 53 (2013).


Geography

Regional vari­a­tion in death sen­tences sug­gests arbi­trari­ness in appli­ca­tion. While one expects to see some vari­a­tion from state to state, giv­en dif­fer­ences in pop­u­la­tion, crime rates and laws, one also expects that in a just sys­tem, (1) death sen­tences con­cen­trat­ed in only one region, and (2) the law of a par­tic­u­lar state would be applied uni­form­ly in that state. However, whether a per­son receives the death penal­ty depends heav­i­ly on where the crime was committed.

In a 2013 report, DPIC found that just 59 coun­ties (few­er than 2% of coun­ties in the coun­try) account­ed for all death sen­tences imposed nation­wide.” These geo­graph­ic dis­par­i­ties can be explained by pros­e­cu­to­r­i­al dis­cre­tion, avail­abil­i­ty of resources to the defense, racial com­po­si­tion of the jury, and polit­i­cal pres­sure on judges up for re-elec­tion. The 2% Death Penalty: How a Minority of Counties Produce Most Death Cases at Enormous Costs to All (2013).


Representation

The qual­i­ty of legal rep­re­sen­ta­tion is relat­ed to the arbi­trary appli­ca­tion of the death penal­ty in that inad­e­quate rep­re­sen­ta­tion con­tributes to mis­takes in capital sentencing.

States vary enor­mous­ly in the qual­i­ty of rep­re­sen­ta­tion they pro­vide to indigent defendants. 

  • In Washington state, one-fifth of the 84 peo­ple who have faced exe­cu­tion in the past 20 years were rep­re­sent­ed by lawyers who had been, or were lat­er, dis­barred, sus­pend­ed or arrest­ed. (Overall, the state’s dis­bar­ment rate for attor­neys is less than 1%.) (Seattle Post-Intelligencer, Aug. 6 – 82001). 
  • In North Carolina, at least 16 death row inmates, includ­ing 3 who were exe­cut­ed, were rep­re­sent­ed by lawyers who have been dis­barred or dis­ci­plined for uneth­i­cal or crim­i­nal con­duct. (Charlotte Observer, Sept. 92000). 
  • In Texas, about one in four death row inmates has been defend­ed by lawyers who have been rep­ri­mand­ed, placed on pro­ba­tion, sus­pend­ed or banned from prac­tic­ing law by the State Bar. (Dallas Morning News, Sept. 102000).
  • In Alabama, about 40 of the approx­i­mate­ly 185 death row inmates – some with­in five months of fil­ing dead­lines for state appeals – do not have coun­sel. (N.Y. Times, July 52001).

In wrong­ful con­vic­tion cas­es in which death-sen­tenced pris­on­ers were exon­er­at­ed between January 2007 and April 2017, over 23% of them had inad­e­quate legal defense. Moreover, lawyers with inad­e­quate resources are often unable to con­duct an ade­quate inves­ti­ga­tion into the facts of the crime and regard­ing mit­i­ga­tion — which is required to explain and human­ize their client. Without the mit­i­gat­ing evi­dence, the jury will be unable to per­form its con­sti­tu­tion­al oblig­a­tion to impose a sen­tence based on a rea­soned moral response to the defen­dan­t’s back­ground, char­ac­ter and crime,’” and the risk of an uncon­sti­tu­tion­al death sen­tence increas­es. Stephen Bright, Counsel for the Poor: The Death Sentence Not for the Worst Crime but for the Worst Lawyer, 103 Yale L. J. 1835 (1994).

Even though the legal stan­dard to show that coun­sel was inef­fec­tive is high, death sen­tences are often reversed due to inad­e­quate coun­sel. In a South Carolina study pub­lished in 2016, over half (26) of the fifty death sen­tences that were reversed in state post-con­vic­tion pro­ceed­ings were due to inef­fec­tive coun­sel. John H. Blume & Lindsey S. Vann, Forty Years of Death: The Past, Present, and Future of the Death Penalty in South Carolina (Still Arbitrary After All These Years), Duke Journal of Constitutional Law & Public Policy, Vol. 11:1&2, at 199 (2016).

People who are well rep­re­sent­ed at tri­al do not get the death penal­ty … I have yet to see a death case among the dozens com­ing to the Supreme Court on eve-of- exe­cu­tion stay appli­ca­tions in which the defen­dant was well rep­re­sent­ed at tri­al.” ‑Justice Ruth Bader Ginsburg (2001)

Juror Misperceptions

The Capital Jury Project stud­ied more than 1,200 jurors from 14 states. Based on this research, Professors Bowers and Foglia con­clude that the con­sti­tu­tion­al­ly man­dat­ed require­ments estab­lished to guide juror dis­cre­tion and to elim­i­nate arbi­trary sen­tenc­ing are not work­ing. The study iden­ti­fied the fol­low­ing constitutional problems:

Premature Decision Making — Interviews with jurors found that approx­i­mate­ly 50% of those inter­viewed decid­ed what the penal­ty should be before the sen­tenc­ing phase of the tri­al. This is before they have heard penal­ty phase evi­dence or received the instruc­tions on how to make the punishment decision.
Bias in Jury Selection — Researchers found that jury selec­tion meth­ods result­ed in dis­pro­por­tion­ate­ly guilt-prone and death-prone juries.
Failure to Understand Jury Instructions — The study found that 45% of jurors failed to under­stand that they were allowed to con­sid­er any mit­i­gat­ing evi­dence dur­ing the sen­tenc­ing phase of the tri­al. In addi­tion, two-thirds of jurors failed to real­ize that una­nim­i­ty was not required for find­ings of mitigation.
Erroneous Beliefs that Death is Required — 44% of jurors said that they believed the death penal­ty was required if the defen­dan­t’s con­duct was heinous, vile or depraved, and near­ly 37% of respon­dents said that the death penal­ty was required if the defen­dant would be dan­ger­ous in the future. The Supreme Court has ruled that no state can require the death penal­ty sole­ly on the grounds that spe­cif­ic aggra­vat­ing cir­cum­stances have been established.
Influence of Race — Researchers revealed that the chances of a death sen­tence in cas­es with a black defen­dant and white vic­tim increase when there are five or more white males on the jury, and the chances decrease when there is at least one black male on the jury. These jurors have very dif­fer­ent per­spec­tives regard­ing lin­ger­ing doubt, defen­dant remorse­ful­ness, and defen­dant future dangerousness.
Underestimating the Death Penalty Alternative — Early find­ings of the study found that most jurors gross­ly under­es­ti­mat­ed the amount of time a defen­dant would serve in prison if not sen­tenced to death, and that the soon­er jurors believed (wrong­ly) a defen­dant would return to soci­ety if not giv­en the death penal­ty, the more like­ly they were to vote for death.

William J. Bowers and Wanda D. Foglia, Still Singularly Agonizing: Law’s Failure to Purge Arbitrariness from Capital Sentencing, 39 Crim. L. Bull. 51 (2003). A list of the pub­li­ca­tions report­ing find­ings from the Capital Jury Project can be found here.