News

How Preconceptions and Bias May Have Led to Wrongful Convictions of West Memphis Three

By Death Penalty Information Center

Posted on Aug 23, 2011 | Updated on Sep 25, 2024

In a recent op-ed in the L.A. Times, Professor Jennifer L. Mnookin (pic­tured) of the UCLA Law School pro­vid­ed an analy­sis of how pre­con­cep­tions and bias­es toward the uncon­ven­tion­al sus­pects known as the West Memphis Three may have led to their wrong­ful con­vic­tions and a death sen­tence in Arkansas in 1994. Because of the gris­ly nature of the mur­ders, inves­ti­ga­tors decid­ed ear­ly on that it was prob­a­bly relat­ed to satan­ic cult rit­u­als. This the­o­ry point­ed them to Damien Echols, who was a self-described Wiccan with an unusu­al taste in clothes and music. Mnookin explained that cog­ni­tive bias­es” — the ten­den­cy of humans to see what they expect to see — played a role in these con­vic­tions. Mnookin wrote, Investigators and pros­e­cu­tors, even when they are try­ing their best to do their jobs, may seek out or take spe­cial notice of evi­dence that con­firms their pri­or beliefs rather than evi­dence that chal­lenges it. And they are like­ly to inter­pret ambigu­ous evi­dence in ways that accord with their pre­con­cep­tions.” For exam­ple, the police inter­viewed Echols’s friend, Jessie Misskelley, who had an IQ of 72, and accept­ed his account of the crime even though it con­tra­dict­ed the evi­dence. Mnookin wrote, The pros­e­cu­tor’s case was based large­ly on char­ac­ter assas­si­na­tion, innu­en­do and the not-very-cred­i­ble tes­ti­mo­ny of the likes of a jail­house snitch and a wit­ness with a mail-order doc­tor­ate. Not a shred of phys­i­cal evi­dence linked any of the young men to the crime scene (and post-con­vic­tion DNA test­ing has also failed to find any bio­log­i­cal evi­dence that they were there).” Mnookin not­ed that at least three of the most com­mon caus­es of wrong­ful con­vic­tion were present in the West Memphis Three case: dubi­ous foren­sic evi­dence, false con­fes­sions and evi­dence from an unre­li­able jail­house infor­mants. She attrib­uted the recent release of the defen­dants part­ly to the work of doc­u­men­tary film­mak­ers who inves­ti­gat­ed the case. Read full op-ed below.

The West Memphis Three’ and com­bat­ing cognitive biases
The case shows we see what we expect to see. That can mean inno­cent peo­ple go to jail while crim­i­nals remain free.

Last week, the West Memphis Three” were released from prison, hav­ing spent half their lives — 18 years — behind bars for crimes they almost cer­tain­ly did­n’t com­mit. So what made pros­e­cu­tors and inves­ti­ga­tors sure they had the right guys, and why were those beliefs, once estab­lished, so hard to reverse?

The crimes for which the three Memphis men were con­vict­ed were bru­tal. Three 8‑year-old Cub Scouts were found dead, hogtied and appar­ent­ly muti­lat­ed. The police decid­ed ear­ly on that it was like­ly the boys had been vic­tims of a satan­ic cult killing, which led them to con­sid­er self-described Wiccan teen Damien Echols, a young man with asym­met­ric black hair, a pale face and odd­ball taste in clothes and music. They hauled in an acquain­tance of his, a minor named Jessie Misskelley, who had an IQ of 72, and inter­viewed him for hours with­out his par­ents or an attor­ney present. Finally, he con­fessed, impli­cat­ing Echols and anoth­er friend, Jason Baldwin.

The con­fes­sion con­firmed what police expect­ed to hear — that Echols was involved — which may be why they accept­ed it at face val­ue. But Misskelley’s account con­tra­dict­ed the evi­dence in mul­ti­ple ways. The time he ini­tial­ly gave for the mur­ders was noon, an hour for which the oth­er teens had an iron­clad ali­bi (they were in school); he said that the oth­er sus­pects raped the boys, but the med­ical evi­dence showed no phys­i­cal trau­ma con­sis­tent with rape and no semen was found in any body cav­i­ty; he said the boys were tied up with a brown rope, when they were actu­al­ly found tied with their own shoe­strings.

An over­ar­ch­ing prob­lem, which this case illus­trates per­fect­ly, is that humans have a ten­den­cy to see what they expect to see. Much psy­cho­log­i­cal research shows that peo­ple are sub­ject to an array of cog­ni­tive bias­es” that affect their eval­u­a­tion of evi­dence, and pros­e­cu­tors and sworn offi­cers are by no means immune to the phe­nom­e­non.

Investigators and pros­e­cu­tors, even when they are try­ing their best to do their jobs, may seek out or take spe­cial notice of evi­dence that con­firms their pri­or beliefs rather than evi­dence that chal­lenges it. And they are like­ly to inter­pret ambigu­ous evi­dence in ways that accord with their pre­con­cep­tions.

Misskelley prompt­ly recant­ed his con­fes­sion. But pros­e­cu­tors nonethe­less pressed the case, and he and the oth­ers were ulti­mate­ly con­vict­ed. The pros­e­cu­tor’s case was based large­ly on char­ac­ter assas­si­na­tion, innu­en­do and the not-very-cred­i­ble tes­ti­mo­ny of the likes of a jail­house snitch and a wit­ness with a mail-order doc­tor­ate. Not a shred of phys­i­cal evi­dence linked any of the young men to the crime scene (and post-con­vic­tion DNA test­ing has also failed to find any bio­log­i­cal evi­dence that they were there). Echols received the death penal­ty, the oth­ers life sen­tences.

That’s how things might have end­ed if two doc­u­men­tary film­mak­ers had­n’t ven­tured to Arkansas to make a film about the case. Initially they thought they would be exam­in­ing a sen­sa­tion­al satan­ic cult killing. But the more research they did, the more they began to doubt that it was a cult killing and that the men who were con­vict­ed were the per­pe­tra­tors. Their film sug­gest­ed the defen­dants had been rail­road­ed, and it led to wide­spread pub­lic­i­ty and high­er-pow­ered legal rep­re­sen­ta­tion.

But noth­ing hap­pened quick­ly. The film came out almost 15 years ago. Even now, to win free­dom, the three men agreed to Alford pleas, where­by they pro­claimed their inno­cence but for­mal­ly plead­ed guilty nonethe­less.

The case demon­strates the need for crim­i­nal jus­tice and evi­den­tiary reforms that would make wrong­ful con­vic­tions less com­mon on the front end. Although releas­ing some frac­tion of those wrong­ful­ly con­vict­ed after­ward is all to the good, it would be even bet­ter if there were few­er of them in the first place.

So what pro­duces wrong­ful con­vic­tions? At least three of the often-seen caus­es were present here: dubi­ous foren­sic sci­ence evi­dence, false con­fes­sions and evi­dence from unre­li­able jail­house infor­mants who often have a strong incen­tive to tell law enforcers what they want to hear.

Cognitive bias helps explain why pros­e­cu­tors can focus on a sus­pect (or three) and fail to see the warn­ing signs that they are head­ed down the wrong path. Cognitive bias is not the same thing as racial bias or per­son­al ani­mus. It’s the habit of our brains to let the first fact we encounter guide our eval­u­a­tion of the sec­ond and the third. One false start can lead to a mis­car­riage of jus­tice more quick­ly than any of us would like to believe.

In this case, once the cops saw Echols as some­thing of a freak, an odd duck who read about witch­craft, liked Metallica and did­n’t exact­ly fit in, the jump from weirdo to like­ly satan­ic cult killer was eas­i­er than it should have been. Facts that did­n’t real­ly prove any­thing were lumped togeth­er with sus­pi­cions and dubi­ous the­o­ries.

We can’t elim­i­nate cog­ni­tive bias­es alto­geth­er; they’re part of how we think. But we can design pro­ce­dures to reduce their effects on inves­ti­ga­tors, pros­e­cu­tors and even jurors. Police depart­ments and pros­e­cu­tors can and should imple­ment mech­a­nisms explic­it­ly designed to com­bat it. For exam­ple, in every major case, an inves­ti­ga­tor or pros­e­cu­tor with no pri­or involve­ment could be asked to review the evi­dence and assess its strengths and weak­ness­es. Even bet­ter would be if this review­er weren’t expect­ed to pro­vide a neu­tral” review but instead were assigned the role of dev­il’s advo­cate, explic­it­ly asked to find the flaws in the pros­e­cu­tors’ the­o­ry. In this case, the film­mak­ers played an equiv­a­lent role, but most defen­dants aren’t so lucky.

If this high-pro­file release helps spur thought­ful atten­tion to the prob­lems of com­bat­ing cog­ni­tive bias in police depart­ments and pros­e­cu­tors’ offices, then some good could still come out of a ter­ri­ble wrong. Bad con­vic­tions harm every­one. Not only do they put inno­cent peo­ple behind bars; they also leave actu­al crim­i­nals — in this case a child mur­der­er — on the streets.

Jennifer L. Mnookin is a pro­fes­sor at UCLA Law School.

(J. Mnookin, The West Memphis Three’ and com­bat­ing cog­ni­tive bias­es,” Los Angeles Times, August 23, 2011). See Innocence.

Citation Guide